| Print | |
We recommend "Landscape" print layout.
There is No Gay Gene
By Carman Bradley
Homosexuals Anonymous, a Christian fellowship for men and women asks,
“Have genes or hormones made you homosexual?”
Some have tried to maintain this argument, but there is little evidence
to support such a view. The HA web
site records many scientific testimonies.
Dr. William Byne and Dr. Bruce Parsons of the Department of Psychiatry
of the Columbia University College of Physicians and Surgeons state:
Recent studies postulate biologic
factors as the primary basis for sexual orientation. However, there is no
evidence at present to substantiate a biologic theory ... Critical review shows
the evidence favoring a biologic theory to be lacking.[i]
After
reviewing the scientific studies on genetics and homosexuality, Masters and
Johnson concluded: "The genetic theory of homosexuality has been
generally discarded today."[ii]
Dr.
C.A. Tripp summarizes the scientific experience regarding hormones and
homosexuality as follows:
A number of clinicians have seen fit over the years to run
their own experiments by administering testosterone to both effeminate and
ordinary homosexuals. The results have been consistent. When there were any behavioral changes at all, the
subjects became more like themselves than ever. Their sex drives were usually
increased and sometimes their effeminate mannerisms as well (when they had
any), but there were never any directional changes in their sexual interests.
From these experiments...it has become abundantly clear that the sex hormones
play a considerable role in powering human sexuality, but they do not control
the direction of it.[iii]
More
recently some have argued that the problem lies in our prenatal hormones. They
suggest that stress during pregnancy may alter the production of sex hormones
in the mother at a crucial time, changing the level of hormones reaching the
brain of the fetus, thus affecting sexual orientation. Here too, however, the available evidence is
against the theory. Thus, researchers
have found that "...in the majority of intersex patients with known
hormone abnormalities, the sexual orientation follows the sex of the
rearing. Consequently, we have to
assume that prenatal hormone conditions by themselves do not rigidly determine sexual
orientation.”[iv]
Dr.
Judd Marmor reported on the work of Richard Green. He writes:
...in a long series of studies on
boys who showed effeminate behavior in childhood has demonstrated that although
over half of these boys do become homosexual, a substantial minority of them do
not. This indicates that gender-discordant children are not born homosexual,
but rather are born with certain behavioral tendencies that, given contributory
environmental factors, can predispose them towards homosexual behavior. Thus, a
little boy whose behavior is effeminate, who does not like competitive
athletics, and who prefers music and art, may be disappointing to a macho
father, who tends to reject the boy and distance himself from him. The mother
may respond by overprotecting her son. Such reactions disturb the boy's
capacity to identify positively with his father and cause him to over identify
with his mother. He may then ultimately develop homosexual erotic responses,
which are reinforced by later experiences.[v]
Dr. John Money says:
With respect to orientation as
homosexual or bisexual, there is no human evidence that prenatal
hormonalization alone, independently of postnatal history, inexorably
preordains either orientation. Rather, neonatal antecedents may facilitate a
homosexual or bisexual orientation, provided the postnatal determinants in the
social and communicational history are also facilitative."[vi]
Dr. Earl D. Wilson writes,
"The disputed evidence for physical causes of male homosexuality is even
weaker when it comes to lesbianism. [vii]
Facts
like these led John DeCecco, editor of the Journal
of Homosexuality and professor of psychology at San Francisco State
University to say:
The idea that people are born into
one type of sexual behavior is foolish.' ... The move towards 'biologizing'
homosexuality, he says, isn't the result of a scientific consensus, but a
political consensus by those eager to label people gay or straight. Homosexuality, he says, is a 'behavior, not
a condition,' and something that some people can and do change, just like they
sometimes change tastes and other personality traits.[viii]
According
to Homosexuals Anonymous, some will
find these truths deeply disturbing.
They rob homosexuals of some favorite excuses. Homosexuals can no longer claim: “I can't help myself. I was born
this way.” These truths mean
homosexuals have to take responsibility for their lives and their actions.”[ix] In doing this, however, these truths give homosexuals the key to
freedom. The scientific truths show GBLTQ that they are not prisoners to cruel
fate or faulty genes or hormones. There
is hope! As Masters and Johnson put it:
When dealing with problems of
sexual preference, it is vital that all health-care professionals bear in mind
that the homosexual man or woman is basically a man or woman by genetic
determination and is homosexually oriented by learned preference.[x]
Dr.
Robert Kronemeyer summarizes his experience:
From my 25 years' experience as a
clinical psychologist, I firmly believe that homosexuality is a learned
response to early painful experiences and that it can be unlearned. For those homosexuals who are unhappy with
their life and find effective therapy, it is 'curable'.[xi]
Joan Laird, professor emerita of Smith College School for
social work, contends:
There is no strong evidence to
date to conclude that lesbians are biologically sexed or gendered any
differently than heterosexual women, and no strong evidence to suggest that
lesbianism is rightly understood as gender inversion or perversion.[xii]
Woman who transition to lesbianism later in life are
particularly problematic for an innate homosexual premise:
One of the
problems with research in this area, which may be used to support the
hypothesis that gayness or lesbianism is biological, is that it is often late
adolescents or adults who are explaining their sexual orientation from a
retrospective position. Kitzinger and Wilkinson point out that ‘this focus on
adolescence is a consequence of an essentialism that assumes a dormant, true
lesbian self waiting to be discovered or revealed at puberty or shortly
thereafter.’ It does little to explain the experiences of women who may change
their self-identity from heterosexual to lesbian in early, mid-, or even late
adulthood. From their research with women who made transitions from
heterosexuality to lesbianism, they concluded that ‘adult women who make such
transitions are no more driven by biology or subconscious urges than they are
when, for instance, they change jobs; such choices could be viewed as
influenced by a mixture of personal re-evaluation, practical necessity,
political values, chance, and opportunity.’ [xiii]
In
1989, the Centers for Disease Control (currently the Centers for Disease
Control and Prevention, or CDC) funded the AIDS Prevention Project of the
Seattle-King County Department of Public Health to develop, implement, and
evaluate interventions targeting non-gay identifying men who have sex with men
(NGI-MSM). Drawing upon personal or
professional knowledge and experience, the project staff defined the risk
population as homosexually active men who did not read the local gay press, did
not participate in local gay events (such as parades or dances), and generally
did not frequent publicly gay establishments (such as bars). However, based on interviews of 79 NGI-MSM
at bathhouses and a movie-sex shop complex, secretive attitudes, risky
behaviors, and denial of risk was evident among these men.[xiv] Project
staff also surveyed individuals who have contact with NGI-MSM but are not
themselves NGI-MSM: employees and managers of adult erotica businesses
(bookstores, video arcades, X-rated theaters), public park groundskeepers,
public and private transportation workers (rest-stop maintenance personnel,
taxicab drivers), bartenders, vice officers, male escorts (prostitutes),
counselors and therapists, and gay and bisexual support group participants.
These interviews clarified points of access to NGI-MSM. The six priority groups were then defined as
follows:
Hustlers, or men who
have sex with men primarily for economic reasons, including adolescents living
on the streets, low-income men, and non-gay-identified professional prostitutes
or escorts.
Closeted (highly
secretive) or coming-out men, including NGI-MSM who generally are not
heterosexually active, but have some compelling reason not to identify as
homosexual. Some men are closeted by choice (to maintain heterosexual privilege
within the general population) and others by circumstance (men in the military
or clergy). This sector also includes men who are in the process of coming to
terms with their sexual orientation.
New Age men or
experimenters, including NGI-MSM who reject conventional notions of sexual
roles and feel free to participate in or experiment with a variety of sexual
activities.
Incarcerated or
formerly incarcerated, including men in and out of jail or prison who may
experience same-sex behavior while incarcerated and who continue to practice
this behavior after release from jail or prison.
People of color or
cultural groups, including NGI-MSM from other sectors who are distinguished by
cultural factors that allow or encourage same-sex behavior among heterosexuals
or that restrict the ability of a member of a particular culture to identify
himself as gay or bisexual if he is involved in same-sex activity.
Heterosexually
identified bisexual men, including married men who have occasional same-sex
encounters, men who have sex with men in all-male institutions (such as
dormitories), and sexually active men who don't necessarily discriminate on the
basis of gender. This sector is a "catch-all" for the majority of
NGI-MSM.[xv]
Chistopher
Hewitt, using both national surveys and surveys of self-identified gay men in
the United States, analyzed the numbers, age distribution, life expectancy, and
marital status of men who have sex with men.
He concluded that five types of behavior could be distinguished: open
preferential, repressed preferential, bisexual, experimental, and situational.
These five categories have different patterns of sexual behavior, and the
numbers in each category are influenced by changing social conditions, in
particular the growth of gay neighborhoods, and public tolerance.[xvi]
Another study that looked at dimensions of sexual experience, as measured on
Kinsey’s Homosexual-Heterosexual Continuum, found significant variety. Male respondents were less apt to consider
themselves exclusively homosexual in their feelings. Some 42 percent of White and more than half of the Black homosexual
males gave themselves a rating of 5 or less (i.e., not exclusively homosexual)
on the scale. A minority of the White
homosexual females (47 per cent) and barely a majority of Black homosexual
females rated themselves exclusively homosexual in both their sexual behaviors
and feelings. Some 75 percent of the
White homosexual females considered themselves more homosexual in their
behaviors than their feelings; 55 percent of White and 41 percent of Black
homosexual females said they had had sex dreams involving sexual activity with
males.[xvii]
These
studies concluded a variety of categories for homosexuality and confirmed that
many could not be considered exclusively homosexual. In the face of such diversity in attraction, Dr. Dean Hammer
says: “We have not found the gene – which we don’t think exists – for sexual
orientation.”[xviii] Neil Whitehead sums the issue as follows:
Hamer knows that any attempt to
argue the existence of a ‘homosexual gene,’ a single, apparently autocratic,
gene governing homosexuality, is nonsense, genetically. There is no single gene governing sexual
preference or any other preference.
There is no gene for smoking, dancing, or making sarcastic remarks.[xix]
More
than 100 genes are involved in IQ in humans because at least 100 separate gene
defects are already known to individually lower IQ. If when many genes are involved,
changes in behavior take place very slowly, over very many generations, how can
homosexuality suddenly appear as it does in a family? The only possible way would be for many recessive “homosexual”
genes to switch on spontaneously and simultaneously very early in the life of
the fetus, and all the “heterosexual” genes to completely switch off. This is extremely unlikely. If many genes were involved the typical genetic
pattern would be a gradual change in the family toward homosexuality – a few
per cent each generation over the course perhaps thirty generations. Similarly, homosexuality would only slowly
disappear in the descendants (if any) of a homosexual person. Whitehead concludes that any other proposed
mechanism is highly speculative. Behaviors
which do change slowly over the generations in a family or society are much
more likely to be genetically influenced or determined, but homosexuality
changes too swiftly to be genetically controlled or influenced by many genes.[xx]
Although
acknowledging that it is highly unlikely the GBLTQ community would accept the
notion of non-heterosexual orientation resulting from genetic mutation,
Whitehead studied the hypothesis. He
concluded, from a biological point of view, that homosexuality does not appear
to be caused by mutation. He explains
why:
Just as many
‘homosexual’ genes would suddenly have to switch on and off if the sudden
appearance and disappearance of homosexuality in families is to be accounted
for, so, many genes would suddenly have to mutate if we want to argue that
homosexuality is caused by mutation.
The chances that even ten genes might spontaneously change from
‘heterosexual’ to ‘homosexual’ by mutation is much less than one in a thousand,
and geneticists would find it inconceivable that hundreds of genes could do so.[xxi]
There is another
difficulty with the mutation theory.
Most conditions caused by mutations affect only a very small proportion
of the general population: about 0.025 per cent of the population or less, in
each case. Altogether, conditions
caused by genetic mutation are found in about only 1 per cent of the total
population. Homosexuality, with its
total incidence (2.2 percent), does not fit plausibly into the category of
genetic diseases because the incidence is too high.[xxii]
Whitehead
asks, “How could genetic homosexuality maintain itself in a population?” In reply he concludes: “genetically
enforced homosexuality (exclusively same-sex sex) would die out of the
population in several generations.”
A gene is retained in the gene pool when an average of at least one
child is born to every adult having that gene (one child per person). As unlikely as it sounds, surveys show that
of persons classifying themselves as exclusively homosexual, one in five has a
child. Moreover, according to surveys,
bisexuals, have an average of 1.25 children each. On its own, that’s enough to replace the adult gene or genes, but
the average total number of children produced by bisexuals and exclusive
homosexuals still comes to less than one per person – 0.9.[xxiii]
No
mainstream geneticist is happy with the idea that genes dictate behavior,
particularly homosexual behavior.
Geneticists G.S. Omenn and A.G. Motulksy said, when they talked about
the difficulties of predicting behavior from gene structure:
The hopelessness of understanding
behavior from simple analytical approaches can be compared to the hopelessness
of seeking linguistic insights by a chemical analysis of a book.[xxiv]
About
1 percent of the adult male population is exclusively homosexual, and about 0.5
per cent of the adult female population is exclusively lesbian at any given
time – a grand mean of 0.7 per cent of the total adult population. Around 2.2 per cent of the total adult
population is GBLTQ.[xxv] The surveys of bisexual incidence come up
with an interesting statistic. Of all
homosexually active males, about 55 per cent are married (which is the average
of a range of surveys finding between 25 per cent and 80 per cent). About 45 per cent of lesbians have been
married.[xxvi]
Those
who accept that homosexuality may not be genetically determined, may then argue
that the behavior is so long-term, so strong, and so resistant to change that
it should be called an instinct or reflex.
But this premise too, needs to be deconstructed. According to Whitehead, we have an
instinctive blinking reflex when something comes near our eyes and even male
ejaculation is a reflex. It can be
tricked by artificial stimulation – even triggered by electric shock.[xxvii] Whitehead draws upon the instincts of
survival and mothering to argue that instinctive responses (reflexes) are not
always rigid and can be changed. He
writes:
Young children have an instinctive
fear of heights. In some experiments
several decades ago, researchers placed a strong sheet glass over a deep recess
created in a level surface and let babies crawl along the sheet glass. All the babies paused in fear at the
apparent edge and retreated. This natural fear of falling is not absent in
potential mountain climbers as babies, but the instinctive fear is abated
through progressive training and experience.
The mothering instinct leads timid ewes and tiny birds to charge humans
and dogs if their babies are threatened.
Mothers are equipped to conceive, carry, and suckle their young. They appear to be natural nurturers. Fathers on the other hand don’t appear to
have the same instinct to nurture.
Surveys usually show that they spend only about one third of the time
with their children that mothers do.
However, are human males biologically programmed or instinctively geared
to be poor nurturers? Where do the good
househusbands and domestic fathers come from? Indeed, if such discrepant
nurturing instincts can be reprogrammed in both females and males, is there not
equal likelihood that such is true of a so-called “homosexual instinct?[xxviii]
Whitehead
cites some revealing experiments:
In an unusual
experiment, scientist Jay Rosenblatt took several-day-old rats and put them in
with virgin females. The females showed
no mothering instincts and of course could not nurse the pups, so the pups
tended to languish. Rosenblatt replaced
the pups each day, and by the sixth day there was an enormous change in the
behavior of the virginal females. They
began to look after the pups, licking them, retrieving them, and even more
astonishingly, lying down as though trying to nurse them. Even though they were not primed by hormonal
changes of pregnancy, the presence of the pups alone was sufficient to trigger
the maternal behavior.
Rosenblatt tried
exactly the same thing with adult male rats.
After six days, the males started behaving just like the virgin females:
licking the pups, retrieving them when they strayed, and even lying down as
trying to nurse them! In other words,
maternal ‘instincts’ were evoked by the presence of the pups in male rats,
sometimes known to eat their infant offspring.[xxix]
The
modern woman who insists that men are quite capable of mothering and nurturing
children appears to have science on her side; fathers are certainly able to
increase the quality time they already spend with their children. “House-husbands” have brought up very young
children with the aid of glass bottles, powdered milk and rubber teats. There are even cases where older men with
hormonal treatment have breast-fed babies.[xxx] Nor is nurturing behavior an over-riding
instinct in human females. Some human
mothers abandon their babies at birth.
Hundreds of thousands of babies are aborted each year. Some women are poor mothers; some men are
successful replacements. Says
Whitehead, “It seems the mothering instinct can be developed or neglected in
a woman, and evoked in a man.” Focusing on our sexuality, some 90 per cent of mankind have a powerful instinct to reproduce. The remaining 10 per cent for various reasons have no urge to personally contribute to perpetuating our species. Anhedonia is an often-temporary state of being turned off from all pleasures, not just sexual ones. In a 1970 study, some 10 per cent of adults saw no prospect of sexual enjoyment with either sex.[xxxi] For many pro-creation is not an overriding drive. Homosexuality cannot reproduce, so homosexuality cannot be considered an instinct to perpetuate the species. Says Whitehead, “If it could be called an instinct, it is no less malleable than any other of the powerful instincts that man experiences, which, we have seen, are subject to a huge degree to man’s will and other environmental influences.”[xxxii] In sum there is no gay gene. Gay theology can neither biblically nor scientifically claim the existence of a gene for homosexual behavior.
Copyright © 2008 StandForGod.Org [i] W. Byne M.D., Ph.D., B. Parsons, M.D., Ph.D, “Human Sexual Orientation: The Biologic Theories Reappraised,” Archives of General Psychiatry, 1993, p. 228. [ii] W.H. Masters, V.E. Johnson, R.C. Kolodny, Human Sexuality, 2nd Ed, (Boston: Little, Brown and Company, 1985), pp.411 and 412. [iii] C.A. Trip, The Homosexual Matrix (New York: McGraw-Hill, 1975), p.12. [iv] A.A. Ehrhardt, H.F.L. Meyer-Bahlburg, "Effects of Prenatal Hormones on Gender-Related Behavior," Science, 1981, p.1316 . [v] J. Marmor, “Homosexuality: Nature versus Nurture,” The Harvard Mental Health Letter, October 1985, p.6. [vi] J. Money, "Sin, Sickness or Status? Homosexual Gender Identity and Psychoneuroendocrinology," American Psychologist, 42, No. 4, April 1987, p. 398. [vii] E.D. Wilson (1988) Counseling and Homosexuality, 42, No.4 (April), p.398 . [viii] K. Painter, “A Biologic Theory for Sexual Preference,” USA Today, March 1, 1989, p.4D. [ix] Homosexuals Anonymous Fellowship Services, www.members.aol.com/HAwebsite/change/change.html, 2/22/01. [x] W.H. Masters, V.E. Johnson, Homosexuality in Perspective (Boston:Little, Brown and Company, 1979). [xi] R. Kronemeyer, Overcoming Homosexuality (New York; Macmillan Publishing, 1980), p.7. [xii] Joan Laird, “Gender in lesbian relationships: Cultural, feminist, and constructionist,” Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, Upland, October 2000. [xiii] Ibid. [xiv] Gary Goldbaum, Thomas Perdue, Donna Higgins, “Non-gay-identifying men who have sex with men: Formative research results from Seattle, Washington,” Public Health Reports, Hyattsville, 1996. [xv] Ibid. [xvi] 237 [xvii] Alan P. Bell and Martin S Weinberg, Homosexualities: A Study of Diversity: Among Men and Women (New York: Simon and Shuster, 1978), pp.57 and 59. [xviii] Neil and Briar Whitehead, My Genes Made Me Do it! (Lafayette, Louisianna: Huntington House, 1999), p.17. [xix] Ibid. [xx] Ibid., pp.20 and 21. [xxi] Ibid., p.23. [xxii] Ibid. [xxiii] Ibid., p.25. [xxiv] Ibid., p.29. [xxv] Ibid., p.39. [xxvi] Ibid. [xxvii] Ibid., p.78. [xxviii] Ibid., pp.77-79. [xxix] Ibid., pp.79 and 80. [xxx] Ibid., p.81. [xxxi] Ibid., p.71. [xxxii] Ibid., p.82. |